The Naturalistic Research How Planning and Conducting: 11 Essential Stages for Success Complete Guide. Planning and conducting naturalistic research involves defining a clear research question, reviewing the existing literature, obtaining ethical approval, and developing the study method.
The Complete Guide to Planning and Conducting Naturalistic Research: 11 Essential Stages for Success
Key steps include access to the field, careful data collection through observation, the use of techniques to ensure reliability and minimize bias, analysis of the results, and the ethical dissemination and presentation of the research findings to participants and the wider scientific community.
What is Naturalistic Research? Understanding the Fundamentals
In many ways the issues in naturalistic research are not exclusive; they apply to other forms of research, for example: identifying the problem and research purposes; deciding the focus of the study; selecting the research design and instrumentation; addressing validity and reliability; ethical issues; approaching data analysis and interpretation. These are common to all research. More specifically Wolcott (1992:19) suggests that naturalistic researchers should address the stages of watching, asking and reviewing, or, as he puts it, experiencing, inquiring and examining.
The 11-Stage Framework for Naturalistic Research Planning
In naturalistic inquiry it is possible to formulate a more detailed set of stages that can be addressed (Hitchcock and Hughes, 1989:57 71; LeCompte and Preissle, 1993; Bogdan and Biklen, 1992):
Stage 1 Locating a field of study.
Stage 2 Addressing ethical issues.
Stage 3 Deciding the sampling.
Stage 4 Finding a role and managing entry into the context.
Stage 5 Finding Informants.
Stage 6 Developing and maintaining relations in the field.
Stage 7 Data collection in situ.
Stage 8 Data collection outside the field.
Stage 9 Data analysis.
Stage 10 Leaving the field.
Stage 11 Writing the Report.
These stages—addressed later in this post are shot through with a range of issues that will affect the research, for example:
- Personal issues (the disciplinary sympathies of the researcher, researcher subjectivities and characteristics. Hitchcock and Hughes (1989:56) indicate that there are several serious strains in conducting fieldwork because the researcher’s own emotions, attitudes, beliefs, values, characteristics enter the research; indeed, the more this happens the less will be the likelihood of gaining the participants’ perspectives and meanings).
- The kinds of participation that the researcher will undertake.
- Issues of advocacy (where the researcher may be expected to identify with the same emotions, concerns and crises as the members of the group being studied and wishes to advance their cause, often a feature that arises at the beginning and the end of the researcher is considered to be a legitimate spokesperson for the group).
- Role relationships.
- Boundary maintenance in the research.
- The maintenance of the balance between distance and involvement.
- Ethical issues.
- Reflexivity recognizes that researchers are incapably part of the social world that they are researching, and, indeed, that this social world is an already interpreted world by the actors, undermining the notion of objective reality. Researchers are in the world and of the world. They bring their own biographies to the research situation and participants behave in particular ways in their presence.
Reflexivity suggests that re searchers should acknowledge and disclose their own selves in the research; they should hold themselves up to the light, echoing Cooley’s (1902) notion of the ‘looking glass self. Highly reflexive researchers will be acutely aware of the ways in which their selectivity, perception, background and inductive processes and paradigms shape the research. They are research instruments.
McCormick and James (1988:191) argue that combating reactivity through reflexivity requires researchers to monitor closely and continually their own interactions with participants, their own reaction, roles, biases, and any other matters that might bias the research.
What are 10 Elements of Naturistic Research
Lincoln and Guba (1985:226–47) set out ten elements in research design for naturalistic studies:
1 Determining a focus for the inquiry.
2 Determining fit of paradigm to focus
3 Determining the fit of the inquiry paradigm to the substantive theory selected to guide the inquiry
4 Determining where and from whom data will be collected.
5 Determining successive phases of the inquiry.
6 Determining instrumentations.
7 Planning data collection and recording modes.
8 Planning data analysis procedures.
9 Planning the logistics:
- prior logistical considerations for the project as a whole
- the logistics of field excursions prior to going into the field
- the logistics of field excursions while in the field
- the logistics of activities following field excursions
- the logistics of closure and termination.
10 Planning for trustworthiness
This can be set out into a sequential, staged approach to planning naturalistic research (see, for example: Schatzman and Strauss, 1973; Delamont, 1992). Spradley (1979) sets out the stages of:
(a) selecting a problem
(b) collecting cultural data
(c) analyzing cultural data
(d) formulating ethnographic hypotheses; writing the ethnography.
More fully, we suggest an eleven stage model.
Stage 1: Locating a Field of Study
Bogdan and Biklen (1992:2) suggest that research questions in qualitative research are not framed by simply operationalizing variables as in the positivist paradigm. Rather, they propose that research questions are formulated in situ and in response to situations observed, i.e. that topics are investigated in all their complexity, in the naturalistic context.
Stage 2: Addressing Ethical Issues
Deyle, Hess and LeCompte (1992:623) identify several critical ethical issues that need to be ad dressed in approaching the research: How does one present oneself in the field? As whom does one present oneself? How ethically defensible is it to pretend to be somebody that you are not in order to:
(a) gain knowledge that you would otherwise not be able to gain
(b) gain and preserve access to places which otherwise you would be unable to gain or sustain such access? The issues here are several. Firstly, there is the issue of informed consent (to participate and for disclosure), whether and how to gain participant assent (see also LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:66).
This uncovers another consideration, namely covert or overt research. On the one hand there is a powerful argument for informed consent. However, the more participants know about the research the less naturally they may behave (ibid.: 108), and naturalism is self-evidently a key criterion of the naturalistic paradigm.
Mitchell (1993) catches the dilemma for re searchers in deciding whether to undertake overt or covert research. The issue of informed con sent, he argues, can lead to the selection of particular forms of research—those where researchers can control the phenomena under investigation—thereby excluding other kinds of research where subjects behave in less controllable, predictable, prescribed ways, indeed where subjects may come in and out of the research over time.
He argues that in the real social world access to important areas of research is prohibited if informed consent has to be sought, for example in researching those on the margins of society or the disadvantaged.
It is in the participants’ own interests that secrecy is maintained as, if secrecy is not upheld, important work may not be done and ‘weightier secrets’ (ibid., p. 54) may be kept which are of legitimate public interest and in the participants’ own interests.
Mitchell makes a powerful case for secrecy, arguing that informed consent may excuse social scientists from the risk of confronting powerful, privileged, and cohesive groups who wish to protect themselves from public scrutiny. Secrecy and informed consent are moot points.
The researcher, then, has to consider her loyalties and responsibilities (LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:106), for example what is the public’s right to know and what is the individual’s right to privacy? (Morrison, 1993).
In addition to the issue of overt or covert research, LeCompte and Preissle (1993) indicate that the problems of risk and vulnerability to participants must be addressed; steps must be taken to prevent risk or harm to participants (non-maleficence—the principle of primum non Nocera).
Bogdan and Biklen (1992:54) extend this to include issues of embarrassment as well as harm to the participants. The question of vulnerability is present at its strongest when participants in the research have their freedom to choose limited, e.g. by dint of their age, by health, by social constraints, by dint of their life style (e.g. engaging in criminality), social accept ability, experience of being victims (e.g. of abuse, of violent crime) (p. 107).
As the authors comment, participants rarely initiate research, so it is the responsibility of the researcher to protect participants. Relationships between researcher and the researched are rarely symmetrical in terms of power; it is often the case that those with more power, information and resources research those with less. A standard protection is often the guarantee of confidentiality, withholding participants’ real names and other identifying characteristics.
The authors contrast this with anonymity, where identity is withheld because it is genuinely unknown (p. 106). The issues of identifiability and traceability are raised. Further, participants might be able to identify themselves in the research report though others may not be able to identify them.
A related factor here is the ownership of the data and the results, the control of the release of data (and to whom, and when) and what rights respondents have to veto the research results.
Patrick (1973) indicates this point at its sharpest, when as an ethnographer of a Glasgow gang, he was witness to a murder; the dilemma was clear—to report the matter (and thereby, also to ‘blow his cover’, consequently endangering his own life) or to stay as a covert researcher.
Bogdan and Biklen (1992:54) add to this discussion the need to respect participants as subjects, not simply as research objects to be used and then discarded.
Stage 3: Deciding the Sampling
In an ideal world the researcher would be able to study a group in its entirety. This was the case in Goffman’s (1968) work on ‘total institutions’—e.g. hospitals, prisons and police forces. It was also the practice of anthropologists who were able to study specific isolated communities or tribes. That is rarely possible nowadays because such groups are no longer isolated or insular.
Hence the researcher is faced with the issue of sampling that is, deciding which people it will be possible to select to rep resent the wider group (however defined). The researcher has to decide the groups for which the research questions are appropriate, the con texts which are important for the research, the time periods that will be needed, and the possible artefacts of interest to the researcher.
In other words decisions are necessary on the sampling of people, contexts, issues, time frames, artefacts and data sources. This takes the discussion beyond conventional notions of sampling, which are confined to issues of sampling populations. In several forms of research sampling is fixed at the start of the study, though there may be attrition of the sample through ‘mortality’ (e.g. people leaving the study).
Mortality is seen as problematic. Ethnographic research regards this as natural rather than a problem. People come into and go from the study. This impacts on the decision whether to have a synchronies investigation occurring at a single point in time, or a diachronic study where events and behavior are monitored over time to allow for change, development, and evolving situations.
In ethnographic inquiry sampling is recursive and ad hoc rather than fixed at the outset; it changes and develops over time. Let us consider how this might happen. LeCompte and Preissle (ibid.: 82–3) point out that ethnographic methods rule out statistical sampling, for a variety of reasons:
- The characteristics of the wider population are unknown
- There are no straightforward boundary markers (categories or strata) in the group
- Generalizability, a goal of statistical methods, is not necessarily a goal of ethnography
- Characteristics of a sample may not be evenly distributed across the sample
- Only one or two subsets of a characteristic of a total sample may be important
- Researchers may not have access to the whole population
- Some members of a subset may not be drawn from the population from which the sampling is intended to be drawn. Hence other types of sampling are required. A criterion-based selection requires the researcher to specify in advance a set of attributes, factors, characteristics or criteria that the study must address.
The task then is to ensure that these appear in the sample selected (the equivalent of a stratified sample). There are other forms of sampling that are useful in ethnographic research (Bogdan and Biklen, 1992:70; LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:69–83), such as:
- Convenience sampling (opportunistic sampling, selecting from whoever happens to be available)
- Critical-case sampling (e.g. people who dis play the issue or set of characteristics in their entirety or in a way that is highly significant for their behavior)
- The norm of a characteristic is identified, then the extremes of that characteristic are located, and finally, the bearers of that extreme characteristic are selected
- Typical case-sampling (where a profile of at tributes or characteristics that are possessed by an ‘average’, typical person or case is identified, and the sample is selected from these typical people or cases)
- Unique-case sampling, where cases that are rare, unique or unusual on one or more criteria are identified, and sampling takes places within these. Here whatever other characteristics or attributes a person might share with others, a particular attribute or characteristic sets that person apart.
- Reputational-case sampling, a variant of extreme-case and unique-case sampling, is where a researcher chooses a sample on the recommendation of experts in the field
- Snowball sampling—using the first inter viewee to suggest or recommend other inter viewees. Patton (1980) identifies six types of sampling that are useful in naturalistic research, including
- Sampling extreme/deviant cases—this is done in order to gain information about unusual cases that may be particularly troublesome or enlightening
- Sampling typical cases—this is done in order to avoid rejecting information on the grounds that it has been gained from special or deviant cases
- Maximum variation sampling—this is done in order to document the range of unique changes that have emerged, often in response to the different conditions to which participants have had to adapt
- Sampling critical cases—this is done in order to permit maximum applicability to others— if the information holds true for critical cases (e.g. cases where all of the factors sought are present), then it is likely to hold true for others
- Sampling politically important or sensitive cases—this can be done to draw attention to the case
- Convenience sampling—this saves time and money and spares the researcher the effort of finding less amenable participants.
Lincoln and Guba (1985:201–2) suggest an important difference between conventional and naturalistic research designs. In the former the intention is to focus on similarities and to be able to make generalizations, whereas in the latter the objective is informational, to provide such a wealth of detail that the uniqueness and individuality of each case can be represented.
To the charge that naturalistic inquiry, thereby, cannot yield generalizations because of sampling flaws the writers argue that this is necessarily though trivially true. In a word, it is unimportant.
Stage 4: Finding a Role and Managing Entry Into the Context
This involves issues of access and permission, establishing a reason for being there, developing a role and a persona, identifying the ‘gate keepers’ who facilitate entry and access to the group being investigated (see LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:100 and 111).
The issue here is complex, for the researcher will be both a member of the group and yet studying that group, so it is a delicate matter to negotiate a role that will enable the researcher to be both participant and observer. The authors comment (p. 112) that the most important elements in securing access are the willingness of researchers to be flexible and their sensitivity to nuances of behaviour and response in the participants.
A related issue is the timing of the point of entry, so that researchers can commence the research at appropriate junctures (e.g. before the start of a programme, at the start of a programme, during a programme, at the end of a programme, after the end of a programme).
The issue goes further than this, for the ethnographer will need to ensure acceptance into the group, which will be a matter of her/his dress, demeanour, persona, age, colour, ethnicity, empathy and identification with the group, language, accent, argot and jargon, willingness to become involved and to take on the group’s values and behaviour etc. (see Patrick’s (1973) fascinating study of a Glasgow gang).
Lofland (1971) suggests that the field re searcher should attempt to adopt the role of the ‘acceptable incompetent’, balancing intrusion with knowing when to remain apart.
Stage 5: Finding Informants
This involves identifying those people who have the knowledge about the society or group being studied. This places the researcher in a difficult position, for she has to be able to evaluate key informants, to decide:
- Whose accounts are more important than others
- Which informants are competent to pass comments
- Which are reliable
- What the statuses of the informants are
- How representative are the key informants (of the range of people, of issues, of situations, of views, of status, of roles, of the group)
- How to see the informants in different settings
- How knowledgeable informants actually are—do they have intimate and expert understanding of the situation
- How central to the organization or situation the informant is (e.g. Marginal or central)
- How to meet and select informants
- How critical the informants are as gatekeepers to other informants, opening up or restricting entry to avenues of inquiry to people (hammersley and atkinson, 1983:73)
- The relationship between the informant and others in the group or situation being studied
The selection and/or relationships with inform ants is problematical; LeCompte and Preissle (1993:95), for example, suggest that the first informants that an ethnographer meets might be self-selected people who are marginal to the group, have a low status, and who, therefore, might be seeking to enhance their own prestige by being involved with the research.
Indeed Lincoln and Guba (1985:252) argue that the re searcher must be careful to use informants rather than informers, the latter possibly having ‘an axe to grind’. Researchers who are working with gatekeepers, they argue, will be engaged in a constant process of bargaining and negotiation.
Stage 6: Developing and Maintaining Relations in the Field
This involves addressing interpersonal and practical issues, for example:
- Building participants’ confidence in the re searcher
- Developing rapport, trust, sensitivity and discretion
- Handling people and issues with which the researcher disagrees or finds objectionable or repulsive
- Being attentive and empathizing
- Being discreet
- Deciding how long to stay.
Spindler and Spindler (1992:65) suggest that ethnographic validity is attained by having the researcher in situ long enough to see things happening repeatedly rather than just once, that is to say, observing regularities.
LeCompte and Preissle (1993:89) suggest that field work, particularly because it is conducted face-to face, raises problems and questions that are less significant in research that is conducted at a distance, including:
(a) How to communicate meaningfully with participants
(b) How they and the researcher might be affected by the emotions evoked in one another, and how to handle these
(c) Differences and similarities between the researcher and the participants (e.g. personal characteristics, power, resources), and how these might affect relationships between parties and the course of the investigation
(d) The researcher’s responsibilities to the participants (qua researcher and member of their community), even if the period of residence in the community is short
(e) How to balance responsibilities to the community with responsibilities to other interested parties. The issue here is that the data collection process is itself socially situated; it is neither a clean, antiseptic activity nor always a straightforward negotiation.
Stage 7: Data Collection in Situ
The qualitative researcher is able to use a variety of techniques for gathering information. There is no single prescription for which data collection instruments to use; rather, the issue here is of ‘fit ness for purpose’ because, as was mentioned earlier, the ethnographer is a methodological omnivore! That said, there are several types of data collection instruments that are used more widely in qualitative research than others.
The researcher can use field notes, participant observation, journal notes, interviews, diaries, life histories, arte facts, documents, video recordings, audio recordings etc.
Several of these are discussed elsewhere in this in my blog posts. Lincoln and Guba (1985:199) distinguish between ‘obtrusive’ (e.g. interviews, observation, non-verbal language) and ‘unobtrusive’ methods (e.g. documents and records), on the basis of whether another human typically is present at the point of data collection. Field notes can be written both in situ and away from the situation.
The popularly used interview technique employed in qualitative interviewing is the semi structured interview, where a schedule is pre pared but it is sufficiently open-ended to enable the contents to be re-ordered, digressions and expansions made, new avenues to be included, and further probing to be undertaken.
Carspecken (1996:159–60) describes how such interviews can range from the interviewer giving bland encouragements, ‘non-leading’ leads, active listening and low-inference paraphrasing to medium- and high-inference paraphrasing. In interviews the researcher might wish to explore further some matters arising from the observations.
In naturalistic research the canons of validity in interviews include: honesty, depth of response, richness of response, and commitment of the interviewee (Oppenheim, 1992).
Lincoln and Guba (1985:268–70) propose several purposes for interviewing, including: present constructions of events, feelings, persons, organizations, activities, motivations, concerns, claims, etc.; reconstructions of past experiences; projections into the future; verifying, amending and extending data.
Further, Silverman (1993:92–3) adds that interviews in qualitative research are useful for:
(a) gathering facts
(b) accessing beliefs about facts
(c) identifying feelings and motives
(d) commenting on the standards of actions (what could be done about situations); (e) present or previous behavior
(f) eliciting reasons and explanations.
Lincoln and Guba (1985) emphasize that the planning of the conduct of the interview is important, including the background preparation, the opening of the interview, its pacing and timing, keeping the conversation going and eliciting knowledge, and rounding off and ending the interview. Clearly, it is important that careful consideration be given to the several stages of the interview.
For example at the planning stage of the interview attention will need to be given to the number (per person), duration, timing, frequency, setting/location, number of people in a single interview situation (e.g. individual or group interviews) and respondent styles (LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:177).
At the implementation stage the conduct of the interview will be important, for example responding to interviewees, prompting, probing, supporting, empathizing, clarifying, crystallizing, exemplifying, summarizing, avoiding censure, accepting.
At the analysis stage there will be several important considerations, for example (ibid.: 195): the ease and clarity of communication of meaning; the interest levels of the participants; the clarity of the question and the response; the precision (and communication of this) of the interviewer; how the interviewer handles questionable responses (e.g. fabrications, untruths, claims made).
The qualitative interview tends to move away from the pre-structured, standardized form and toward the open-ended or semi-structured interview, as this enables respondents to project their own ways of defining the world. It permits flexibility rather than fixity of sequence of discussions, and it also enables participants to raise and pursue issues and matters that might not have been included in a pre-devised schedule (Denzin, 1970; Silverman, 1993).
In addition to interviews, Lincoln and Guba (1985) discuss data collection from non-human sources, including:
1 Documents and records (e.g. archival records, private records). These have the attraction of being always available, often at low cost, and being factual. On the other hand they may be unrepresentative; they may be selective, lack objectivity, be of unknown validity, and may possibly be deliberately deceptive (see Finnegan, 1996).
2 Unobtrusive informational residues. These include artefacts, physical traces, and a variety of other records. Whilst they frequently have face validity, and whilst they may be simple and direct, gained by non-interventional means (hence reducing the problems of reactivity), they may also be very heavily inferential, difficult to interpret, and may contain elements whose relevance is questionable.
Stage 8: Data Collection Outside the field
In order to make comparisons and to suggest explanations for phenomena, researchers might find it useful to go beyond the confines of the groups in which they occur. That this is a thorny issue is indicated in the following example. Two students are arguing very violently and physically in a school. At one level it is simply a fight between two people.
However, this is a common occurrence between these two students as they are neighbor’s outside school and they don’t enjoy positive amicable relations as their families are frequently feuding. The two households have been placed next door to each other by the local authority because the authority has taken a decision to keep together families who are very poor at paying for their local housing rent (i.e. a ‘sink’ estate).
The local authority has taken this decision because of a government policy to keep together disadvantaged groups so that targeted action and interventions can be more effective, meeting the needs of whole com munities as well as individuals. The issue here is: how far out of a micro situation does the researcher need to go to understand that micro-situation?
This is an imprecise matter but it is not insignificant in educational research (e.g. it underpinned:
(a) The celebrated work by Bowles and Gintis (1976) on schooling in capitalist America, in which the authors suggested that the hidden curricula of schools were preparing students for differential occupational futures that perpetuated an in egalitarian capitalist system
(b) Research on the self-fulfilling prophecy (Hurn, 1978)
(c) Work by Pollard (1985:110) on the social world of the primary school, where everyday interactions in school were preparing students for the individualism, competition, achievement orientation, hierarchies and self-reliance that characterize mass private consumption in wider society
(d) Delamont’s (1981) advocacy that educationists should study similar but different institutions to schools (e.g. hospitals and other ‘total’ institutions) in order to make the familiar strange (see also Erickson, 1973).
Stage 9: Data Analysis
This involves organizing, accounting for, and explaining the data; in short, making sense of the data in terms of the participants’ definitions of the situation, noting patterns, themes, categories and regularities. Typically in qualitative research, data analysis commences during the data collection process. There are several reasons for this, and these are discussed below.
At a practical level, qualitative research rap idly amasses huge amounts of data, and early analysis reduces the problem of data overload by selecting out significant features for future focus.
Miles and Huberman (1984) suggest that careful data display is an important element of data reduction and selection. ‘Progressive focusing’, according to Parlett and Hamilton (1976), starts with the researcher taking a wide angle lens to gather data, and then, by sifting, sorting, reviewing and reflecting on them the salient features of the situation emerge.
These are then used as the agenda for subsequent focusing. The process is akin to funneling from the wide to the narrow. At a theoretical level a major feature of qualitative research is that analysis commences early on in the data collection process so that theory generation can be undertaken (LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:238).
The authors (pp. 237–53) advise that researchers should set out the main outlines of the phenomena that are under investigation. They then should assemble chunks or groups of data, putting them together to make a coherent whole (e.g. through writing summaries of what has been found).
Then they should painstakingly take apart their field notes, matching, contrasting, aggregating, comparing and ordering notes made. The intention is to move from description to explanation and theory generation. Becker and Geer (1960) indicate how this might proceed:
- Comparing different groups simultaneously and over time
- Matching the responses given in interviews to observed behaviour
- An analysis of deviant and negative cases
- Calculating frequencies of occurrences and responses
- Assembling and providing sufficient data that keeps separate raw data from analysis.
Data Analysis Steps
For clarity, the process of data analysis can be portrayed in a sequence of seven steps:
Step 1 Establish units of analysis of the data, indicating how these units are similar to and different from each other.
Step 2 Create a ‘domain analyses.
Step 3 Establish relationships and linkages between the domains.
Step 4 Making speculative inferences.
Step 5 Summarizing. Step 6 Seeking negative and discrepant cases.
Step 7 Theory generation.
The following pages address each of these steps.
Step 1: establish units of analysis of the data, indicating how these units are similar to and different from each other The criterion here is that each unit of analysis (category conceptual, actual, classification element, cluster, issue) should be as discrete as possible whilst retaining fidelity to the integrity of the whole, i.e. that each unit must be a fair rather than a distorted representation of the context and other data.
The creation of units of analysis can be done by ascribing codes to the data (Miles and Huberman, 1984). This is akin to the process of ‘unitizing’ (Lincoln and Guba, 1985:203). Codes define categories; they are astringent, pulling together a wealth of material into some order and structure. They keep words as words; they maintain context specificity.
At this stage the codes are essentially descriptive and might include (Bogdan and Biklen, 1992:167–72): situation codes; perspectives held by subjects; ways of thinking about people and objects; process codes; activity codes; event codes; strategy codes; relationship and social structure codes; methods codes.
However, to be faithful to the data, the codes themselves derive from the data responsively rather than being created pre-ordinately. Hence the researcher will go through the data ascribing codes to each piece of datum. The code is a word or abbreviation that is sufficiently close to that which it is de scribing that the researcher can see at a glance what it means (in this respect it is unlike a number).
For example, the code ‘trust’ might refer to a person’s trustworthiness; the code ‘power’ might refer to the status or power of the person in the group. Miles and Huberman advise that codes should be kept as discrete as possible and that coding should start earlier rather than later as late coding enfeebles the analysis.
It is possible, they suggest, for as many as ninety codes to be held in the working memory whilst going through data, though clearly, there is a process of iteration and reiteration whereby some codes that are used in the early stages of coding might be modified subsequently and vice versa, necessitating the researcher to go through a data set more than once to ensure consistency, refinement, modification and exhaustiveness of coding (some codes might become redundant, others might need to be broken down into finer codes).
By coding up the data the researcher is able to detect frequencies (which codes are occurring most commonly) and patterns (which codes occur together).
Hammersley and Atkinson (1983:177–8) pro pose that the first activity here is to read and re read the data to become thoroughly familiar with them, noting also any interesting patterns, any surprising, puzzling or unexpected features, any apparent inconsistencies or contradictions (e.g. between groups, within and between individuals and groups, between what people say and what they do).
Step 2: Create a ‘Domain Analysis’
This involves grouping the units into domains, clusters, groups, patterns, themes and coherent sets to form domains. A domain is any symbolic category that includes other categories (Spradley, 1979:100).
At this stage it might be useful for the researcher to recode the data into domain codes, or to review the codes used to see how they naturally fall into clusters, perhaps creating overarching codes for each cluster. Hammersley and Atkinson (1983) show how items can be assigned to more than one category, and, indeed, see this as desirable as it maintains the richness of the data.
This is akin to the process of ‘categorization’ (Lincoln and Guba, 1985), putting ‘unitized’ data to provide descriptive and inferential information. Spradley (1979) suggests that establishing domains can be achieved by four analytic tasks:
(a) Selecting a sample of verbatim interview and field notes
(b) Looking for the names of things
(c) Identifying possible terms from the sample
(d) Searching through additional notes for other items to include.
He identifies six steps to achieve these tasks:
(i) Select a single semantic relationship
(ii) Prepare a domain analysis sheet
(iii) Select a sample of statements from respondents
(iv) Search for possible cover terms and included terms that fit the semantic relationship identified
(v) Formulate structural questions for each domain identified
(vi) List all the hypothesized domains. Domain analysis, then, strives to dis cover relationships between symbols (ibid.: 157)
Step 3: Establish Relationships and Linkages Between the Domains
This process ensures that the data, their rich ness and ‘context-groundedness’ are retained. Linkages can be found by identifying confirming cases, by seeking ‘underlying associations’ (LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:246) and connections between data subsets.
Step 4: Making Speculative Inferences
This is an important stage, for it moves the research from description to inference. It requires the researcher, on the basis of the evidence, to posit some explanations for the situation, some key elements and possibly even their causes. It is the process of hypothesis generation or the setting of working hypotheses that feeds into theory generation.
Step 5: Summarizing
By this stage the researcher will be in a position to write a summary of the main features of the situation that have been researched so far. The summary will identify key factors, key issues, key concepts and key areas for subsequent investigation.
It is a watershed stage during the data collection, as it pinpoints major themes, issues and problems that have arisen from the data to date (responsively) and suggests avenues for further investigation.
The concepts used will have been a combination of those derived from the data themselves and those inferred by the re searcher (Hammersley and Atkinson, 1983:178). By this stage the researcher will have gone through the preliminary stages of theory generation. Patton (1980) sets these out for qualitative data:
- Finding a focus for the research and analysis
- Organizing, processing, ordering and checking data
- Writing a qualitative description or analysis
- Inductively developing categories, typologies, and labels
- Analyzing the categories to identify where further clarification and cross-clarification are needed
- Expressing and typifying these categories through metaphors (see also pitman and maxwell, 1992:747)
- Making inferences and speculations about relationships, causes and effects.
Bogdan and Biklen (1992:154–63) identify several important items that researchers need to address at this stage, including: forcing yourself to take decisions that will focus and narrow the study and decide what kind of study it will be; developing analytical questions; using previous observational data to inform subsequent data col lection; writing reflexive notes and memos about observations, ideas, what you are learning; trying out ideas with subjects; analyzing relevant literature whilst you are conducting the field research; generating concepts, metaphors and analogies and visual devices to clarify the research.
Step 6: Seeking Negative and Discrepant Cases
In theory generation it is important to seek not only confirming cases but to weigh the significance of discontinuing cases. LeCompte and Preissle (1993:270) suggest that because interpretations of the data are grounded in the data themselves, results that fail to support an original hypothesis are neither discarded nor dis-credited; rather, it is the hypotheses themselves that must be modified to accommodate these data.
Indeed Erickson (1992:208) identifies progressive problem-solving as one key aspect of ethnographic research and data analysis. LeCompte and Preissle (1993:250–1) define a negative case as an exemplar which disconfirms or refutes the working hypothesis, rule or explanation so far. It is the qualitative researcher’s equivalent of the positivist’s null hypothesis.
The theory that is being developed becomes more robust if it ad dresses negative cases, for it sets the boundaries to the theory; it modifies the theory, it sets parameters to the applicability of the theory. Discrepant cases are not so much exceptions to the rule (as in negative cases) as variants of the rule (ibid.: 251).
The discrepant case leads to the modification or elaboration of the construct, rule or emerging hypothesis. Discrepant case analysis requires the researcher to seek out cases for which the rule, construct or explanation cannot account or with which they will not fit, i.e. they are neither exceptions nor contra dictions, they are simply different!
Step 7: Theory Generation
Here the theory derives from the data it is grounded in the data and emerges from it. As Lincoln and Guba (1985:205) argue, grounded theory must fit the situation that is being re searched. By going through the previous sections, particularly the search for confirming, negative and discrepant cases, the researcher is able to keep a ‘running total’ of these cases for a particular theory.
The researcher also generates alternative theories for the phenomena under investigation and performs the same count of con firming, negative and discrepant cases. Lincoln and Guba (ibid.: 253) argue that the theory with the greatest incidence of confirming cases and the lowest incidence of negative and discrepant cases is the most robust. There are several procedural tools for analyzing qualitative data.
Statistical Methods of Data Analysis
LeCompte and Preissle (ibid.: 253) see analytic induction, constant comparison, typological analysis and enumeration (discussed above) as valuable tools for the qualitative researcher to use in analyzing data and generating theory. Analytic induction is a term and process that was introduced by Znaniecki (1934) deliberately in opposition to statistical methods of data analysis.
LeCompte and Preissle (1993:254) suggest that the process is akin to the several steps set out above, in that:
(a) Data are scanned to generate categories of phenomena
(b) Relationships between these categories are sought
(c) Working typologies and summaries are written on the basis of the data examined
(d) These are then re fined by subsequent cases and analysis
(e) Negative and discrepant cases are deliberately sought to modify, enlarge or restrict the original explanation/theory.
Strategies for Observations
Denzin (1970:192) uses the term ‘analytical induction’ to describe the broad strategy of participant observation that is set out below:
- A rough definition of the phenomenon to be explained is formulated.
- A hypothetical explanation of that phenomenon is formulated.
- One case is studied in the light of the hypothesis, with the object of determining whether or not the hypothesis fits the facts in that case.
- If the hypothesis does not fit the facts, either the hypothesis is reformulated or the phenomenon to be explained is redefined, so that the case is excluded.
- Practical certainty may be attained after a small number of cases has been examined, but the discovery of negative cases disproves the explanation and requires a reformulation.
- This procedure of examining cases, redefining the phenomenon, and reformulating the hypothesis is continued until a universal relationship is established, each negative case calling for a redefinition of a reformulation.
Stages In Analytical Induction
A more deliberate seeking of discontinuing cases is advocated by Bogdan and Biklen (1992:72) where they enumerate five main stages in analytic induction:
Step 1 In the early stages of the research a rough definition and explanation of the particular phenomenon is developed.
Step 2 This definition and explanation is examined in the light of the data that are being collected during the research.
Step 3 If the definition and/or explanation that have been generated need modification in the light of new data (e.g. if the data do not fit the explanation or definition) then this is under taken.
Step 4 A deliberate attempt is made to find cases that may not fit into the explanation or definition.
Step 5 The process of redefinition and reformulation is repeated until the explanation is reached that embraces all the data, and until a generalized relationship has been established, which will also embrace the negative cases.
Constant comparison, LeCompte and Preissle (1993:256) opine, combines the elements of inductive category coding (see above) with simultaneously comparing these with the other events and social incidents that have been observed and coded over time and location.
This enables social phenomena to be compared across categories, where necessary, giving rise to new dimensions, codes and categories. Glaser (1978) indicates that constant comparison can proceed from the moment of starting to collect data, to seeking key issues and categories, to discovering recurrent events or activities in the data that become categories of focus, to expanding the range of categories.
This process can continue during the writing-up process (which should be continuous), so that a model or explanation of the phenomena can emerge that accounts for fundamental social processes and relationships. In constant comparison data are compared across a range of situations, times, groups of people, and through a range of methods.
Constant Comparison Method
The process resonates with the methodological notion of triangulation. Glaser and Strauss (1967:105–6) suggest that the constant comparison method involves four stages:
(1) Comparing incidents and data that are applicable to each category, comparing them with previous incidents in the same category and with other data that are in the same category
(2) Integrating these categories and their properties
(3) Bounding the theory
(4) Setting out the theory
Typological analysis is essentially a classificatory process (LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:257) wherein data are put into groups, subsets or categories on the basis of some clear criterion (e.g. acts, behaviour, meanings, nature of participation, relationships, settings, activities).
It is the process of secondary coding (Miles and Huberman, 1984) where descriptive codes are then drawn together and put into subsets. Typologies are a set of phenomena that represent subtypes of a more general set or category (Lofland, 1970).
Dimension or Key Characteristics
Lazarsfeld and Barton (1951) suggest that a typology can be developed in terms of an underlying dimension or key characteristic. In creating typologies Lofland insists that the researcher must:
(a) Deliberately assemble all the data on how a participant addresses a particular issue what strategies are being employed
(b) Disaggregate and separate out the variations between the ranges of instances of strategies
(c) Classify these into sets and sub sets
(d) Present them in an ordered, named and numbered way for the reader
Issues in Analyzing and Interpreting
Lincoln and Guba (1985:354–5) urge the re searcher to be mindful of several issues in analyzing and interpreting the data, including:
(a) Data overload
(b) The problem of acting on first impressions only
(c) The availability of people and information (e.g. how representative these are and how to know if missing people and data might be important)
(d) The dangers of only seeking confirming rather than discontinuing instances
(e) The reliability and consistency of the data and confidence that can be placed in the results. These are significant issues in addressing reliability, trustworthiness and validity in the research.
The essence of this approach, that theory emerges from and is grounded in data, is not without its critics. For example, Silverman (1993:47) suggests that it fails to acknowledge the implicit theories which guide research in its early stages (i.e. data are not theory neutral but theory-saturated) and that it might be strong on providing categorizations without necessarily having explanatory potential. These are caveats that should feed into the process of reflexivity in qualitative research, perhaps.
Stage 10: Leaving the Field
The issue here is how to terminate the research, how to terminate the roles adopted, how (and whether) to terminate the relationships that have built up over the course of the research, and how to disengage from the field in ways that bring as little disruption to the group or situation as possible (LeCompte and Preissle, 1993:101).
Stage 11: Writing the Report
Delamont (1998) notes the shift in emphasis in much research literature, away from the con duct of the research and towards the reporting of the research. It is often the case that the main vehicle for writing naturalistic research is the case study, whose ‘trustworthiness’ (Lincoln and Guba, 1985:189) is defined in terms of credibility, transferability, depend ability and conformability—discussed.
Case studies are useful in that they can provide the thick descriptions that are useful in ethnographic research, and can catch and portray to the reader what it is like to be involved in the situation (ibid.: 214). As the writers comment (p. 359), the case study is the ideal instrument for ‘emic’ inquiry.
Guideline Writing a Case Study
It also builds in and builds on the tacit knowledge that the writer and reader bring to the report, and, thereby, takes seriously their notion of the ‘human instrument’ in research, indicating the interactions of researcher and participants. Lincoln and Guba provide several guidelines for writing case studies (ibid.: 365–6):
- The writing should strive to be informal and to capture informality;
- As far as possible the writing should report facts except in those sections where interpretation, evaluation and inference are made explicit;
- In drafting the report it is more advisable to opt for over-inclusion rather than under-inclusion;
- The ethical conventions of report writing must be honored, e.g. Anonymity, non-traceability;
- The case study writer should make clear the data that gave rise to the report, so the readers have a means of checking back for reliability and validity and inferences;
- A fixed completion date should be specified.
Steps In Writing the Ethnographic Report
Spradley suggests nine practical steps that can be followed in writing ethnography:
Step 1 Select the audience.
Step 2 Select the thesis.
Step 3 Make a list of topics and create an outline of the ethnography.
Step 4 Write a rough draft of each section of the ethnography.
Step 5 Revise the outline and create subheadings.
Step 6 Edit the draft.
Step 7 Write an introduction and a conclusion.
Step 8 Re-read the data and report to identify examples.
Step 9 Write the final version.
Clearly there are several other aspects of case study reporting that need to be addressed.
Read More:
https://nurseseducator.com/didactic-and-dialectic-teaching-rationale-for-team-based-learning/
https://nurseseducator.com/high-fidelity-simulation-use-in-nursing-education/
First NCLEX Exam Center In Pakistan From Lahore (Mall of Lahore) to the Global Nursing
Categories of Journals: W, X, Y and Z Category Journal In Nursing Education
AI in Healthcare Content Creation: A Double-Edged Sword and Scary
Social Links:
https://www.facebook.com/nurseseducator/
https://www.instagram.com/nurseseducator/
https://www.pinterest.com/NursesEducator/
https://www.linkedin.com/company/nurseseducator/
https://www.linkedin.com/in/nurseseducator/
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Afza-Lal-Din
https://scholar.google.com/citations?hl=en&user=F0XY9vQAAAAJ
I found the connections between gut health and hearing quite intriguing, especially regarding the role of Xifaxan. Given its ability to reduce bacterial overgrowth and potentially decrease inflammation, could Xifaxan play a subtle role in managing conditions like tinnitus that may stem from gut-related issues? I came across an insightful article that elaborates on this perspective: https://hearinghealthclinics.com/understanding-xifaxan-a-practical-guide-from-a-clinical-perspective. What are your thoughts on this connection?
Очистка в СПб! Студии, Поместья, Кабинеты. Профессиональный сервис по доступным предложениям. Позвольте себе расслабиться! Обратитесь к нам очистку не откладывая! Заходите https://uborka-top24.ru